Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why Minimal Guidance During Instruction Does Not Work: An Analysis of the Failure of Constructivist, Discovery, Problem-Based, Experiential, and Inquiry-Based Teaching. Educational Psychologist, 41(2), 75–86

An opinion / review paper arguing against Constructivism, Inquiry-based learning, Situated learning, and other methods which emphasize students solving authentic problems in authentic settings, often using Cognitive scaffolding-laden versions of the authentic disciplinary methods.

I’m quite sympathetic to constructivist framings (How can I reconcile my views on inquiry learning and memory systems?), but I think the authors are correct in their central criticism that advocates systematically underrate the role of memory (Many people view memory as unimportant to deep creative work) and the mechanisms of cognition. On the other hand, I accuse the authors of systematically ignoring the role of motivation and meaning (Enabling environments’ activities directly serve an intrinsically meaningful purpose).

Q. Three main theoretical criticisms of constructivism and its peers?
A. 1) expertise relies on LTM, while constructivism mostly ignores it; 2) low-guidance tasks overload WM (per CLT); 3) epistemology != pedagogy (teaching of an inquiry-driven discipline isn’t necessarily best done via inquiry)

Q. My primary criticism of KSC?
A. They’re totally disinterested in motivation, attitude, feeling, meaning. (see also Kuhn’s response)

Commentaries

In 2007’s volume 42 issue 2, there are three commentaries in reply, and then a follow-up from the original trio responding to the commentaries.

Schmidt, H. G., Loyens, S. M. M., Van Gog, T., & Paas, F. (2007). Problem-Based Learning is Compatible with Human Cognitive Architecture: Commentary on Kirschner, Sweller, and Clark (2006). Educational Psychologist, 42(2), 91–97

Hmelo-Silver, C. E., Duncan, R. G., & Chinn, C. A. (2007). Scaffolding and Achievement in Problem-Based and Inquiry Learning: A Response to Kirschner, Sweller, and Clark (2006). Educational Psychologist, 42(2), 99–107

  • Like Schmidt et al, they argue that PBL / IL aren’t minimally guided. Scaffolding is provided, but sometimes only when students need to know.
    • KSC’s reply is similar to above: scaffolding is good, but why not consider “full” scaffolding? I think they’re right. It’s ideological. Or reflective of an unvoiced tradeoff between authenticity/motivation and efficacy/clarity.
  • They point out that PBL and IL are designed to emphasize “disciplinary epistemologies and investigative strategies” (in addition to content knowledge)
    • KSC also accuse Hmelo-Silver et al for failing to ground their preferred methods in any explicit cognitive architecture, and for emphasizing general problem-solving skills which don’t appear to exist.
    • “The techniques favored by Hmelo-Silver et al. (2007) were developed in an era when it was thought that the central component of human cognition was not knowledge in long-term memory, but rather the ability to devise novel, general problem-solving and thinking strategies. IL was intended to foster this skill. The failure over many decades to isolate a single, novel, teachable, general problem-solving or thinking strategy has inhibited that pursuit.”
    • KSC argue that it’s not clear we can teach such strategies directly; or at least, H-S offers no evidence that we can.
  • Thy offer many examples of empirical studies in which PBL and IL demonstrate learning benefits.
    • KSC retort that these studies aren’t well-controlled enough; they’re comparing totally different curricula with different students in different places. “We might expect that meta-analyses of such studies would yield mixed, ambiguous results, precisely the findings of the meta-analyses cited by Hmelo-Silver et al.”

Kuhn, D. (2007). Is Direct Instruction an Answer to the Right Question? Educational Psychologist, 42(2), 109–113.

  • The author claims that KSC are asking the wrong question. The real challenge is figuring out what to learn, not how to learn it.
    • KSC reject this premise; irrespective of the answer to “what to learn?” we’ll still need good answers to “how?” (I agree.)
  • The author says that we should emphasize “the skills of knowledge acquisition”
    • KSC say that it’s not clear what those skills are, how to teach them, or how they might relate to cognitive architecture.
    • I don’t think that retort is strong enough. If those skills exist, it’s obvious that we should emphasize them, which would mean figuring out what they are and how they work mechanistically. The stronger concern (which I think KSC share; they express something similar to H-S) is that these skills don’t exist. People who seem more skilled at knowledge acquisition actually just have a big store of knowledge and schemas, so it’s easier for them to pick new stuff up. Maybe on the margin they have some reading strategies and so on, but those aren’t what’s decisive.
  • The author suggests that content knowledge in science is less important than attitudes and values. “Are scientific topics worth learning about, knowing about, or inquiring about any more deeply? And, as the motivation theorists highlight, another critical question: Am I someone who is competent to engage in such learning?”
    • KSC reject this out of hand as, basically, a difference of opinion.
    • I think this objection is actually quite important, though I view it as complementary to KSC’s concerns. Reminds me a lot of Dan Meyer’s arguments.
  • Most compellingly, the author emphasizes motivation.
    • “Activities centered on inquiry and argument enable students to appreciate the power and utility of these skills as they practice them.”
    • “…key to predicting students’ performance is examining what it is that they are undertaking to learn in school, what they think it means, how they construe the meaning of this material in relation to themselves, and whether they can see it as worth learning”
    • “…students need to learn what it is scientists do and why they bother to do it. Students can develop that understanding only by engaging…in the practice of science.”
    • KSC see this as irrelevant. I see it as central and quite appropriate.
  • Kuhn cites a bunch of empirical papers and KSC ignore them; I didn’t dig into them.

For another very interesting later response, see Schwartz, D. L., Lindgren, R., & Lewis, S. (2009). Constructivism in an age of non-constructivist assessments. In Constructivist instruction: Success or failure? (pp. 34–61). Routledge\/Taylor & Francis Group.

For another, more balanced review, see Lee, H. S., & Anderson, J. R. (2013). Student Learning: What Has Instruction Got to Do With It? Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 445–469

See also much related criticism in Anderson, J. R., Reder, L. M., Simon, H. A., Ericsson, K. A., & Glaser, R. (1998). Radical Constructivism and Cognitive Psychology. Brookings Papers on Education Policy, 1, 227–278.

Last updated 2024-04-26.